Social Grooming in Bats: Are Vampire Bats Exceptional? (2024)

1. Seyfarth RM. A model of social grooming among adult female monkeys. J Theor Biol. 1977;65(4):671–98. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

2. Silk JB, Beehner JC, Bergman TJ, Crockford C, Engh AL, Moscovice LR, et al. The benefits of social capital: close social bonds among female baboons enhance offspring survival. Proc R Soc B. 2009;276(1670):3099–104. 10.1098/rspb.2009.0681 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

3. Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. The evolutionary origins of friendship. Annu Rev Psychol. 2012;63:153–77. Available: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100337 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

4. Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. Affiliation, empathy, and the origins of theory of mind. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2013;110:10349–56. 10.1073/pnas.1301223110 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

5. Seyfarth RM, Silk JB, Cheney DL. Social bonds in female baboons: the interaction between personality, kinship and rank. Anim Behav. 2014;87:23–9. [Google Scholar]

6. Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. Social cognition. Anim Behav. 2015;103:191–202. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.01.030 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

7. Brent LJ, Chang SW, Gariépy JF, Platt ML. The neuroethology of friendship. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2014;1316(1):1–17. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

8. Brent LJ. Friends of friends: are indirect connections in social networks important to animal behaviour?Anim Behav. 2015;103:211–22. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

9. Gero S, Gordon J, Whitehead H. Individualized social preferences and long-term social fidelity between social units of sperm whales. Anim Behav. 2015;102:15–23. [Google Scholar]

10. Connor RC, Krützen M. Male dolphin alliances in Shark Bay: changing perspectives in a 30-year study. Anim Behav. 2015;103(0):223–35. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.02.019 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

11. Carter GG, Wilkinson GS. Cooperation and conflict in the social lives of bats In: Adams R, Pedersen S, editors. Bat Evolution, Ecology, and Conservation. New York: Springer Science Press; 2013. p. 225–42. [Google Scholar]

12. Archie EA, Moss CJ, Alberts SC. The ties that bind: genetic relatedness predicts the fission and fusion of social groups in wild African elephants. Proc R Soc B. 2006;273(1586):513–22. Available: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3361 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

13. Smith JE, Powning KS, Dawes SE, Estrada JR, Hopper AL, Piotrowski SL, et al. Greetings promote cooperation and reinforce social bonds among spotted hyaenas. Anim Behav. 2011;81(2):401–15. Available: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.11.007 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

14. Gersick AS, Cheney DL, Schneider JM, Seyfarth RM, Holekamp KE. Long-distance communication facilitates cooperation among wild spotted hyaenas, Crocuta crocuta. Anim Behav. 2015;103:107–16. Available: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.02.003 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

15. Holekamp KE, Dantzer B, Stricker G, Shaw Yoshida KC, Benson-Amram S. Brains, brawn and sociality: a hyaena's tale. Anim Behav. 2015;103(0):237–48. Available: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.01.023 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

16. Fraser ON, Bugnyar T. The quality of social relationships in ravens. Anim Behav. 2010;79(4):927–33. Available: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.01.008 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

17. Boeckle M, Bugnyar T. Long-Term Memory for Affiliates in Ravens. Curr Biol. 2012:1–6. Available: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.03.023 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

18. Fraser ON, Bugnyar T. Reciprocity of agonistic support in ravens. Anim Behav. 2012;83(1):171–7. Available: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.10.023 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

19. Massen JJ, Szipl G, Spreafico M, Bugnyar T. Ravens intervene in others’ bonding attempts. Curr Biol. 2014;24(22):2733–6. 10.1016/j.cub.2014.09.073 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

20. Massen JJ, Pašukonis A, Schmidt J, Bugnyar T. Ravens notice dominance reversals among conspecifics within and outside their social group. Nature Comm. 2014;5. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

21. Miklósi Á, Kubinyi E, Topál J, Gácsi M, Virányi Z, Csányi V. A simple reason for a big difference: wolves do not look back at humans, but dogs do. Curr Biol. 2003;13(9):763–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

22. Hare B, Tomasello M. Human-like social skills in dogs?Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9(9):439–44. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

23. Emery NJ, Clayton NS. The mentality of crows: convergent evolution of intelligence in corvids and apes. Science. 2004;306(5703):1903–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

24. Emery NJ, Seed AM, von Bayern AMP, Clayton NS. Cognitive adaptations of social bonding in birds. Phil Trans R Soc B. 2007;362(1480):489–505. Available: 10.1098/rstb.2006.1991 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

25. Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. Grooming, alliances and reciprocal altruism in vervet monkeys. Nature. 1984;308(5959):541–3. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

26. Barrett L, Henzi SP, Weingrill T, Lycett JE, Hill RA. Market forces predict grooming reciprocity in female baboons. Proc R Soc B. 1999;266(1420):665–70. [Google Scholar]

27. Hemelrijk CK, Ek A. Reciprocity and interchange of grooming and support in captive chimpanzees. Anim Behav. 1991;41:923–35. [Google Scholar]

28. Muroyama Y. Exchange of grooming for allomothering in female patas monkeys. Behav. 1994;128:103–19. [Google Scholar]

29. Watts DP. Reciprocity and interchange in the social relationships of wild male chimpanzees. Behav. 2002;139:343–370. [Google Scholar]

30. Gomes CM, Mundry R, Boesch C. Long-term reciprocation of grooming in wild West African chimpanzees. Proc R Soc B. 2009;276:699–706. Available: 10.1098/rspb.2008.1324 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

31. Schino G, Aureli F. Grooming reciprocation among female primates: a meta-analysis. Biol Lett. 2008;4(1):9–11. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

32. Fruteau C, Lemoine S, Hellard E, van Damme E, Noë R. When females trade grooming for grooming: Testing partner control and partner choice models of cooperation in two species of primates. Anim Behav. 2011;81:1223–30. [Google Scholar]

33. Tiddi B, Aureli F, di Sorrentino EP, Janson CH, Schino G. Grooming for tolerance? Two mechanisms of exchange in wild tufted capuchin monkeys. Behav Ecol. 2011;22(3):663–9. [Google Scholar]

34. Jaeggi AV, De Groot E, Stevens JM, Van Schaik CP. Mechanisms of reciprocity in primates: testing for short-term contingency of grooming and food sharing in bonobos and chimpanzees. Evol Hum Behav. 2013;34(2):69–77. [Google Scholar]

35. de Waal FB. Food transfers through mesh in brown capuchins. J Comp Psychol. 1997;111(4):370–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

36. de Waal FBM. The chimpanzee's service economy: Food for grooming. Evol Hum Behav. 1997;18(6):375–86. [Google Scholar]

37. Fruteau C, Voelkl B, van Damme E, Noë R. Supply and demand determine the market value of food providers in wild vervet monkeys. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2009;106:12007–12. 10.1073/pnas.0812280106 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

38. Dunbar R. Functional significance of social grooming in primates. Folia Primatol. 1991;57:121–31. [Google Scholar]

39. Aureli F, Preston SD, de Waal F. Heart rate responses to social interactions in free-moving rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): a pilot study. J Comp Psychol. 1999;113(1):59 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

40. Shutt K, Maclarnon A, Heistermann M, Semple S. Grooming in Barbary macaques: better to give than to receive?Biol Lett. 2007;3(3):231–3. Available: 10.1098/rsbl.2007.0052 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

41. Boccia ML, Reite M, Laudenslager M. On the physiology of grooming in a pigtail macaque. Physiol Behav. 1989;45(3):667–70. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

42. Russell EM. Social behaviour and social organization of marsupials. Mamm Rev. 1984;14(3):101–54. [Google Scholar]

43. Forand KJ, Marchinton RL. Patterns of social grooming in adult white-tailed deer. Amer Mid Nat. 1989:357–64. [Google Scholar]

44. Wood MT. Social grooming patterns in two herds of monozygotic twin dairy cows. Anim Behav. 1977;25:635–42. [Google Scholar]

45. Kimura R. Mutual grooming and preferred associate relationships in a band of free-ranging horses. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1998;59(4):265–76. [Google Scholar]

46. Ophir AG, Crino OL, Wilkerson QC, Wolff JO, Phelps SM. Female-directed aggression predicts paternal behavior, but female prairie voles prefer affiliative males to paternal males. Brain Behav Evol. 2008;71(1):32 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

47. Stopka P, Graciasová R. Conditional allogrooming in the herb-field mouse. Behav Ecol. 2001;12(5):584–9. [Google Scholar]

48. Kutsukake N, Clutton-Brock T. Social functions of allogrooming in cooperatively breeding meerkats. Anim Behav. 2006;72(5):1059–68. [Google Scholar]

49. Kutsukake N, Clutton-Brock TH. Grooming and the value of social relationships in cooperatively breeding meerkats. Anim Behav. 2010;79(2):271–9. [Google Scholar]

50. Russell JK. Altruism in coati bands: nepotism or reciprocity In: Wasser SK, editor. Social Behavior of Female Vertebrates. New York: Academic Press; 1983:263–90. [Google Scholar]

51. Hirsch BT, Stanton MA, Maldonado JE. Kinship shapes affiliative social networks but not aggression in ring-tailed coatis. PLOS One. 2012;7(5): e37301 Available: 10.1371/journal.pone.0037301 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

52. Matoba T, Kutsukake N, Hasegawa T. Head rubbing and licking reinforce social bonds in a group of captive African lions, Panthera leo. PLOS One. 2013;8(9):e73044 10.1371/journal.pone.0073044 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

53. Gill SA. Strategic use of allopreening in family-living wrens. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2012;66(5):757–63. [Google Scholar]

54. Radford AN. Post-allogrooming reductions in self-directed behaviour are affected by role and status in the green woodhoopoe. Biol Lett. 2012;8(1):24–7. 10.1098/rsbl.2011.0559 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

55. Wilkinson GS. Social grooming in the common vampire bat, Desmodus rotundus. Anim Behav. 1986;34:1880–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

56. Ancillotto L, Serangeli MT, Russo D. Spatial proximity between newborns influences the development of social relationships in bats. Ethol. 2012;118(4):331–40. [Google Scholar]

57. Kerth G, Almasi B, Ribi N, Thiel D, Lüpold S. Social interactions among wild female Bechstein's bats (Myotis bechsteinii) living in a maternity colony. Acta Ethol. 2003;5(2):107–14. [Google Scholar]

58. Ortega J, Maldonado JE. Female interactions in harem groups of the Jamaican fruit-eating bat, Artibeus jamaicensis (Chiroptera: Phyllostomidae). Acta Chiropterol. 2006;8(2):485–95. [Google Scholar]

59. Arnold BD, Wilkinson GS. Individual specific contact calls of pallid bats (Antrozous pallidus) attract conspecifics at roosting sites. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2011;65(8):1581–93. [Google Scholar]

60. Carter GG, Wilkinson GS. Food sharing in vampire bats: reciprocal help predicts donations more than relatedness or harassment. Proc R Soc B. 2013;280:20122573 Available: 10.1098/rspb.2012.2573 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

61. Aureli F, Schaffner CM, Boesch C, Bearder SK, Call J, Chapman CA, et al. Fission‐fusion dynamics. Curr Anthro. 2008; 49: 627–654. [Google Scholar]

62. Wilkinson GS. The social organization of the common vampire bat: I. Pattern and cause of association. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1985;17:111–121. [Google Scholar]

63. Kerth G, Perony N, Schweitzer F. Bats are able to maintain long-term social relationships despite the high fission-fusion dynamics of their groups. Proc R Soc B. 2011;278(1719):2761–7. Available: 10.1098/rspb.2010.2718 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

64. Wilkinson GS. Reciprocal food sharing in the vampire bat. Nature. 1984;308:181–4. [Google Scholar]

65. Carter G, Wilkinson G. Does food sharing in vampire bats demonstrate reciprocity?Comm Integr Biol. 2013;6(6):e25783 10.4161/cib.25783 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

66. Baron G, Stephan H, Frahm HD. Comparative Neurobiology in Chiroptera. Basel: Birkhäuser Verlag; 1996. 1596 p. [Google Scholar]

67. Bhatnagar K. The brain of the common vampire bat, Desmodus rotundus murinus (Wagner, 1840): a cytoarchitectural atlas. Braz J Biol. 2008; 68:583–599. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

68. Kudo H, Dunbar RIM. Neocortex size and social network size in primates. Anim Behav. 2001; 62(4), 711–722. [Google Scholar]

69. Byrne RW, Corp N. Neocortex size predicts deception rate in primates. Proc R Soc B. 2004; 271(1549), 1693–1699. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

70. ter Hofstede HM, Fenton MB. Relationships between roost preferences, ectoparasite density, and grooming behaviour of neotropical bats. J Zool. 2005;266(04):333–40. [Google Scholar]

71. Patterson BD, Dick CW, Dittmar K. Parasitism by bat flies (Diptera: Streblidae) on neotropical bats: effects of host body size, distribution, and abundance. Parasitol Res. 2008;103(5):1091–100. 10.1007/s00436-008-1097-y [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

72. Canty A, Ripley B. boot: Bootstrap R (S-Plus) Functions. R package version 1.5. 2015.

73. Carter GG, Logsdon R, Arnold B, Menchaca A, Medellin RA. Adult vampire bats produce contact calls when isolated: acoustic variation between species, colonies, and individuals. PLOS One. 2012;7(6):e38791 Available: 10.1371/journal.pone.0038791 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

74. Carter GG, Skowronski M, Faure P, Fenton MB. Antiphonal calling allows individual discrimination in white-winged vampire bats. Anim Behav. 2008;76:1343–55. [Google Scholar]

75. Carter GG, Fenton MB, Faure P. White-winged vampire bats (Diaemus youngi) exchange contact calls. Can J Zool. 2009;87:604–8. [Google Scholar]

76. Williams CF. Social organization of the bat Carollia perspicillata (Chiroptera: Phyllostomidae). Ethol. 1986;71:265–82. [Google Scholar]

77. Knörnschild M, Feifel M, Kalko EK. Male courtship displays and vocal communication in the polygynous bat Carollia perspicillata. Behav. 2014;151(6):781–98. [Google Scholar]

78. Fernandez AA, Fasel N, Knörnschild M, Richner H. When bats are boxing: aggressive behaviour and communication in male Seba's short-tailed fruit bat. Anim Behav. 2014;98:149–56. [Google Scholar]

79. Knörnschild M, Feifel M, Kalko EK. Mother–offspring recognition in the bat Carollia perspicillata. Anim Behav. 2013;86(5):941–8. [Google Scholar]

80. August PV (1979) Distress calls in Artibeus jamaicensis: ecology and evolutionary implications In: Eisenberg JF, editor. Vertebrate ecology in the northern Neotropics. Washington DC: Smithsonian Institution Press; 1979. pp. 151–159. [Google Scholar]

81. Ortega J, Maldonado JE, Wilkinson GS, Arita HT, Fleischer RC. Male dominance, paternity, and relatedness in the Jamaican fruit‐eating bat (Artibeus jamaicensis). Mol Ecol. 2003;12(9):2409–15. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

82. Ortega J, Guerrero JA, Maldonado JE. Aggression and tolerance by dominant males of Artibeus jamaicensis: strategies to maximize fitness in harem groups. J Mammal. 2008;89(6):1372–8. [Google Scholar]

83. Kwiecinski GG, Griffiths TA. Rousettus egyptiacus. Mamm. Species. 1999:1–9. [Google Scholar]

84. Prat Y, Taub M, Yovel Y. Vocal learning in a social mammal: Demonstrated by isolation and playback experiments in bats. Sci Adv. 2015; 1(2), e1500019. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

85. Roberts G, Thomas NS. Development of cooperative relationships through increasing investment. Nature. 1998; 394(6689),175–179. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Social Grooming in Bats: Are Vampire Bats Exceptional? (2024)

FAQs

Social Grooming in Bats: Are Vampire Bats Exceptional? ›

Vampire bats spent 3.7% of their awake time social grooming (95% CI = 1.5-6.3%), whereas bats of the other species spent 0.1-0.5% of their awake time social grooming.

What is the social behavior of a vampire bat? ›

SOCIAL BEHAVIOR

Within the roost, females tend to form groups of 8-12 individuals that roost close together on a regular basis. The males roost separately, with each defending a small territory from other males. Desmodus can breed year-round, but each female breeds just once per year.

What makes a vampire bat special? ›

Unlike other species of bats, vampire bats can walk, run, and jump, which helps them attach to their prey. Heat sensors on their noses help them find a good spot on an animal's body to feed. And strong hind legs and a special thumb help them take off after feeding.

Do vampire bats groom each other? ›

Vampire bats frequently groom each other by licking each other's fur, wings and face (Wilkinson, 1986).

Are vampire bats social altruistic animals? ›

Now, a new study, published March 19 in Current Biology, shows that the world's only known blood-sucking mammals develop trust with unrelated individuals first by grooming each other, then eventually regurgitating blood to share—an act of altruism for a species that must eat every three days.

Do bats groom each other? ›

Like cats, bats clean themselves.

Some, like the Colonial bat, even groom each other.

Are vampire bats social or solitary? ›

Stable social bonds in group-living animals can provide greater access to food. A striking example is that female vampire bats often regurgitate blood to socially bonded kin and nonkin that failed in their nightly hunt. Food-sharing relationships form via preferred associations and social grooming within roosts.

Why are vampire bats social? ›

The researchers suspect that the bats join their friends during the hunt to help each other. They think the bats might exchange information about prey position or access to an open wound for feeding. Vampire bats live in the warmer areas of Central and South America.

How intelligent are vampire bats? ›

Vampire bats are probably the most intelligent of all bats as well. Each bat must compile and remember a detailed mental map of its ever changing flight routes to where it has found food in the past. After feeding they are often so bloated with concentrated blood that they can scarcely even fly.

What are 3 interesting facts about vampire bats? ›

Fun Facts
  • Vampire bats have fewer teeth than any other bat because they do not have to chew their food.
  • Along with flying, vampire bats can run, jump, and hop with great speed, using their chest muscles to fling themselves skyward.

Do bats groom themselves? ›

Bats are extremely clean, sometimes grooming, licking, and scratching themselves for hours.

What do vampire bats do for their closest friends? ›

Social bonds among vampire bats as they roost in trees include grooming one another and regurgitating blood meals for hungry pals.

Can bats show affection? ›

However, vampire bats are also very cuddly, at least with one another. Female bats cluster together for warmth, share food, and groom their cuddle-mates by licking each other's fur. Being groomed can reduce stress, lower heart rate, and promote cooperation.

Are vampire bats friendly? ›

Are vampire bats friendly? They are very friendly, while they lap up your blood.

What happens when two vampire bats meet? ›

In theory, reciprocal relationships should develop gradually, to reduce the risk of helpers being exploited. In a classic case of reciprocity, vampire bats share blood with starving roost-mates. Now it transpires they share food only after first having established grooming relationships.

What animal is friends with bats? ›

Bats can easily find all the food they need by hanging out with deer, and, by consuming hundreds to thousands of insects a night, they might provide substantial relief for besieged deer! Interactions between species where both individuals benefit are known as mutualisms.

What is the social structure of a bat? ›

In European and North American bats, males are usually solitary and females are usually social during the summer; nevertheless, both sexes often use the same type of roost. For example, in Bechstein's bats, both solitary males and social females roost in tree cavities under natural conditions (Kerth and Morf 2004).

How are bats social? ›

Most bats live in colonies and many of them have incredibly complex social lives for which good communication is necessary. Scientists believe many species can sing; some can recognize each other's voices; they have dialects; they can alert others to dangers; and they form intense bonds with their young.

What are some bat behaviors? ›

During the day bats sleep in trees, rock crevices, caves, and buildings. Bats are nocturnal (active at night), leaving daytime roosts at dusk. Upon leaving their roost, bat fly to a stream, pond, or lake where they dip their lower jaw into the water while still in flight and take a drink.

Top Articles
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Kieth Sipes

Last Updated:

Views: 6699

Rating: 4.7 / 5 (47 voted)

Reviews: 94% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Kieth Sipes

Birthday: 2001-04-14

Address: Suite 492 62479 Champlin Loop, South Catrice, MS 57271

Phone: +9663362133320

Job: District Sales Analyst

Hobby: Digital arts, Dance, Ghost hunting, Worldbuilding, Kayaking, Table tennis, 3D printing

Introduction: My name is Kieth Sipes, I am a zany, rich, courageous, powerful, faithful, jolly, excited person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.